HOME
pISSN 2005-9159
eISSN 2093-0569

Original Article

Korean J Pain 2015; 28(4): 265-274

Published online October 31, 2015 https://doi.org/10.3344/kjp.2015.28.4.265

Copyright © The Korean Pain Society.

The Impact of Educational Status on the Postoperative Perception of Pain

Sophocles Lanitis*, Christina Mimigianni*, Demetris Raptis, Gionous Sourtse, George Sgourakis, and Constantine Karaliotas

2nd Surgical Department and Unit of Surgical Oncology "Korgialenio-Benakio", Red Cross Athens General Hospital, Greece.

*Department of Anaesthesiology "Korgialenio-Benakio", Red Cross Athens General Hospital, Greece.

Correspondence to: Sophocles Lanitis. 2nd Surgical Department and Unit of Surgical Oncology "Korgialenio-Benakio", Red Cross Athens General Hospital, 1 Athanasaki st. 11526, Athens, Greece. Tel: +30-213-206-8735, Fax: +30-213-206-8616, drlanitis@yahoo.com

Received: January 29, 2015; Revised: June 25, 2015; Accepted: July 2, 2015

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution Non-Commercial License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/4.0/), which permits unrestricted non-commercial use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Background

Postoperative (PO) pain interferes with the recovery and mobilization of the surgical patients. The impact of the educational status has not been studied adequately up to now.

Methods

This prospective study involved 400 consecutive general surgery patients. Various factors known to be associated with the perception of pain including the educational status were recorded as was the preoperative and postoperative pain and the analgesia requirements for the 1st PO week. Based on the educational status, we classified the patients in 3 groups and we compared these groups for the main outcomes: i.e. PO pain and PO analgesia.

Results

There were 145 patients of lower education (junior school), 150 patients of high education (high school) and 101 of higher education (university). Patients of lower education were found to experience more pain than patients of higher education in all postoperative days (from the 2nd to the 6th). No difference was identified in the type and quantity of the analgesia used. The subgroup analysis showed that patients with depression and young patients (< 40 years) had the maximum effect.

Conclusions

The educational status may be a significant predictor of postoperative pain due to various reasons, including the poor understanding of the preoperative information, the level of anxiety and depression caused by that and the suboptimal request and use of analgesia. Younger patients (< 40), and patients with subclinical depression are mostly affected while there is no impact on patients over 60 years old.

Keywords: Analgesia, Anxiety, Depression, Educational status, General surgery, Postoperative pain, Predictive value of tests, Questionnaires

Pain is one of the most common symptoms affecting surgical patients during the postoperative period. Since pain is very subjective and a highly personal experience, it is affected by many factors related to each individual's special characteristics [1]. Despite the evolution of anesthesia and analgesics, one third of the patients still complained about moderate to severe pain after surgery [2,3]. Effectively controlling the postoperative pain is desirable for various reasons. The postoperative course of the patients should be as comfortable as possible and cause the least possible distress. Failure to adequately control pain may lead to unfavorable postoperative course, long hospitalization, and even chronic pain [3,4,5].

Many efforts have been made to identify predictors of postoperative pain [1,3]. Unfortunately, the factor of "educational status" has not been studied extensively. It has, however, been found that low education is associated with a higher incidence of painful conditions [6]. The few studies that assessed the association of the educational status with the postoperative pain were inconclusive. They either showed no association [7,8] or in one case a negative association ( i.e. the higher the education, the lower the pain) but this was based only on 40 patients [9]. To our knowledge though, there is no study that actually addressed directly this issue since the aforementioned studies had a different aim. Moreover, we are not aware of any other study performed in a large number of general surgery patients like ours.

On the other hand, a lot of effort has been made to educate the patients preoperatively about the operation and the associated sequellae in order to effectively control the pain. Even in cancer patients, this approach seems to be effective as this was shown in a recent meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials (RCT) [10]. Similar results were presented previously by others [11]. Generally speaking, the structured preoperative education has been found to have a beneficial effect on postoperative pain, anxiety, and recovery; hence, this approach has been used extensively to alleviate the postoperative complications [12]. Various methods of education have been used in an attempt to universally educate the patients before the operation, and these depend both on the delivery method and the ability of the recipient comprehend and interpret the necessary information.

We believe that the educational status of the patients is a significant predictor of pain. In the absence of sufficient evidence, we designed this study in order to assess if there is indeed an association, and which other factors should be taken into account preoperatively in order to have the optimal results in terms of pain management. Our aim was to identify the group of patients at risk for a higher pain score, based on their educational status in order to find a way to offset this handicap, if any.

1. Statistics

The statistical analysis was carried out using SPSS for Windows version 19 software package (Statistical Package for Social sciences; Inc, Chicago, IL).

For categorical variables, we used the Pearson Chi-square test and Fisher's exact test. In tables bigger than 2 × 2, when the null hypothesis for independence was rejected, (i.e. the variables in the x axis were dependent from the variables in the y axis) we compared the observed frequencies with the expected frequencies (standardized residual) for each cell in order to identify which comparisons led to significant outcomes. In order to do this, we compared the size of the standardized residuals to the critical values that correspond to an alpha of 0.05 (±1.96) to identify the observed values which were significantly more or less than the expected values.

For the continuous variable, we used independent sample t-test for equality of means to assess if there were any statistically significant differences between the means of the outcome values.

For comparisons involving more than 2 groups, we used analysis of variance (ANOVA) to identify statistically significant differences between the means of the outcome values of the groups.

Having rejected the Null hypothesis (i.e. statistically significant effect in ANOVA), we further applied follow-up tests (post-hoc) to assess which groups were different from which other groups or to test various other focused hypotheses. The "Tukey's" and "Bonferroni" post hoc tests were used. We considered significant differences those with P < 0.05. Binary logistic regression was used to assess if there were significant predictors of binary outcomes.

1. Depression (this predictor was found to be significant in both the multivariate and the univariate analysis)

1) No depression

When we compared Group A with Group B for patients who did not have depression, we found that there were still significant differences in the weight, height and length of scar but no difference in their weight and the anxiety score. More specifically, patients of Group A were older (66.6 years vs. 50.5 years, P < 0.001), shorter (165.7 cm vs. 169.8 cm, P < 0.001) and had a longer scar (6.9 cm vs. 5.4 cm, P = 0.003). There were no differences in the preoperative and postoperative pain. More people of group B were smokers (41.1% vs. 23.3%, P = 0.002) but otherwise there were no differences in the sex, in the use of analgesics or narcotics before the operation, in the experience of previous operations, in the percentage of urgent operations, in the type of the scar, and in the anxiety score. Furthermore, there were no differences in the type and quantity of the analgesia required postoperatively.

2) Depression

People of Group A were older (73 years vs. 52.3 years, P < 0.001) and had a longer scar (9.5 cm vs. 4.4 cm, P = 0.001) but otherwise there were no differences in the weight, height, preoperative pain and anxiety score. Despite the fact that there were no differences during the 1st PO day, significant differences were observed during the postoperative period in terms of the pain intensity between the groups. More specifically, a higher pain score was observed in Group A for the 2nd PO day (3.7 vs. 2.0, P = 0.12), the 3rd PO day (2.6 vs. 1.5, P = 0.049), the 4th PO day (1.9 vs. 0.7, P = 0.015), the 5th PO day (1.5 vs. 0.4, P = 0.011) and the 6th PO day (1.3 vs. 0.3, P = 0.013).

There were no differences in the sex, use of analgesics/narcotics before the operation, smoking, previous surgery, urgent operation, and anxiety. Furthermore, there were no differences in the type and quantity of the analgesia required postoperatively.

2. Anxiety (this predictor was found to be significant only in univariate analysis)

1) No anxiety

There were more patients with depression in Group A (11.7% vs. 4.5%, P = 0.02) but otherwise no other significant differences were found in the qualitative factors. Moreover, no differences were observed in the type and quantity of the analgesia required postoperatively. Patients of Group A were older (66.4 years vs. 50.2 years, P < 0.001), shorter and with a longer scar (7.6 vs. 5.7, P < 0.001) and had a higher depression score (3.8 vs. 2.8, P = 0.002) but there were no other differences in the quantitative factors. In terms of postoperative pain, there was a higher pain score during the 2nd PO day for Group A (2.7 vs. 2.0, P = 0.026) but otherwise no other differences were observed.

2) Anxiety

Patients of Group A were older (73 years vs. 52.9 years, P < 0.001), and shorter (161.9 cm vs. 166.2 cm, P = 0.015) but there were not any other differences in the other quantitative factors or in the level of postoperative pain. There were no differences between the groups for any of the qualitative factors including the postoperative use of analgesia.

3. Age (this predictor was found to be significant only in univariate analysis)

Since age was found to be significantly different between the 2 groups, we did a subgroup analysis based on 3 age groups: the patients under 40 years old (N = 84), patients from 40-60 years old (N = 128) and patients over 60 years (N = 188).

1) Age under 40 years

There were no significant differences in any of the factors studied hence, no confounding factors. Moreover, there was no difference in the preoperative pain. On the other hand, significant differences were observed between the 2 groups during the postoperative day 2 and 3. More specifically, Group A had a higher pain score on the 2nd PO day (3.7 vs. 1.6, P = 0.005) and on the 3rd PO day (2.4 vs. 1.0, P = 0.016). Moreover, Group A required more and stronger analgesia for the 1st (P = 0.029), 2nd (P = 0.035) and 4th PO days (P = 0.005) while there were no differences for the 3rd, 5th and 6th PO day.

2) Age 40-60 years

There were no significant differences in any of the factors studied. Moreover, there was no difference in the preoperative pain and on postoperative pain. On the other hand, there were significant differences in the analgesia required. Group A required more and stronger analgesia for the 1st (P = 0.013), 3rd (P = 0.045), 4th (P = 0.038) and 6th PO days (P = 0.027), while there were no differences for the 2nd, and 5th PO day.

3) Age over 60 years

There were no significant differences in any of the factors studied. Moreover, there was no difference in the preoperative pain, postoperative pain or the analgesia required.

In this study we found that the educational status is a significant predictor of pain. Moreover, we found that patients who had lower education were more prone to experience depression or anxiety. All these 3 factors seem to be dependent on each other.

Despite the fact that there are no similar studies in the literature (based on general surgery patients), there is data in the literature supporting our findings. More specifically, a study on heart failure patients showed that patients with low education experienced more pain than patients of higher education during the first 6 months [16].

On the other hand, it has been shown that there is indeed a strong association between educational status and mood disorders. A study investigating this association in more than 10,000 people from the general population showed that the relative risk for anxiety was significantly higher for persons with lower education and this was more pronounced in females. Moreover, the same was true for depression where it was found that the lower the education, the higher the relative risk [17]. In the aforementioned study, people of lower education were found to have differences in their socioeconomic status and the material factors. The differences in the socioeconomic status (SES) in patients with lower education are most likely responsible for the higher incidence of mood disorders in this population.

Low SES has been constantly related to mood disorders [18,19]. Nevertheless, education is an independent factor influencing rather than being the result of the SES, since it is offered free in most countries (at least in Greece) and precedes occupation and income, which are factors influencing the SES thereafter. Obtaining a good education increases the odds to succeed in western society and achieve goals associated with good emotional well being. Studies have shown that there is a cumulative protective effect of high education on mood disorders, especially depression, and a subsequent advantage on physical health. The HUNT study investigated if the higher education protects against anxiety and depression. In this study, like us, they used the HADS to assess the mood conditions. It was shown that the lowest educational level was associated with higher anxiety/depression scores and this is in line with our findings [20]. The association of low education with anxiety is important since anxiety, when excessive, may have a negative impact on the course and recovery of the surgical patients. It has been widely supported that the anxiety about the anticipated pain and the feeling that there is lack of control of that, sensitizes the individual to the pain stimuli by increasing the reactive pain [21,22]. It has been shown that gaining control or understanding better the anticipated condition leads to better adaptation and improved tolerance of it, since the condition may be rendered less threatening. This can be achieved by properly educating the patients and depends on the level of education and the way the medical personnel approaches the patients. Educating patients about the medical actions that will take place should not be a passive process with no confirmation of whether the information is comprehended. On the contrary, it should be an interactive process, with confirmation that learning has taken place [21]. Nevertheless, the patients can be classified in two categories according to the desired level of preoperative education and the amount of information they want to be given: the vigilant and the avoidant [23]. Therefore, information should be tailored to suit each individual's general level of understanding, education and cultural background [24]. Eventually, patients who understand more about their condition will comply with their care needs and will facilitate their recovery and minimize the hospitalization need [21].

A systematic review on the impact of the preoperative education on the postoperative pain showed that this intervention is indeed effective but none of the studies compared the effects of preoperative education and 'no form of education,' and therefore the actual impact may have been underestimated. Nevertheless, sufficient evidence was found to support the effectiveness of preoperative patient education in the improvement of postoperative outcomes [25].

In our study it is important that there was no difference in the preoperative pain between the groups and that the groups were not different in most of the known confounding factors such as sex, use of narcotics/analgesics, previous experience with surgery, urgency of the operation and weight. Patients of lower education experienced more pain than the rest of the patients during the 1st PO week. There was a correlation of the educational status with psychological factors; i.e. anxiety and depression which are known predictors of postoperative pain. It is possible that these factors may have contributed to the results of our study. Indeed, when we did a subgroup analysis including only the patients who did not score high enough for depression (normal), the differences in the intensity of pain between the 2 groups were eliminated. In this subgroup analysis the difference in the mean age between the 2 groups was still significant as was the length of the scar. It is important that there were no differences in the anxiety level which could have acted as a confounding factor. Based on these findings, we may conclude that the educational status is not a "handicap" if we manage to alleviate or control the "subclinical" depression of these patients, and this should be our aim as clinicians. This can be achieved with a proper structured preoperative education and a psychiatric consultation for those patients who score high in the HADS.

On the other hand, when we included only the patients who had a level of depression, the lower education group had a higher pain score on the PO day 2 onwards and this matched the overall results of the analysis. Based on this, we may assume that patients who have lower education cannot effectively manage their subclinical depression during the perioperative period, as opposed to patients of higher education. On the other hand, if the patients have no psychological issues, the educational status has no impact on the perception of pain. When we did a subgroup analysis based on the state of anxiety, we did not find any further differences between the groups, hence the anxiety only mildly affected the results.

Based on the fact that one out of four patients with a lower level of education had some form of depression, this should be taken into account when trying to educate the patients preoperatively.

Although all patients were informed about their operation in the same way by the same trained medical personnel, it is possible that patients of lower education either failed to correctly interpret the information given or hesitated to ask for clarifications when in doubt. This may explain the fact that despite the fact that they had higher pain scores, they did not receive additional or stronger analgesia available on demand by the nursing staff.

Finally, age is known to influence the perception of pain and indeed there were differences between the educational groups. When we stratified the patients according to their age we found that the impact of educational status was very pronounced in the younger patients (i.e. < 40 years), less pronounced in the middle age group (40-60 years) and was eliminated in the elderly (> 60 years). Interestingly when we stratified the patients in 3 age groups, all differences between the groups in all confounding factors were eliminated and the two groups were perfectly matched. Henceforth, in this subgroup analysis all differences found in the main outcomes can only be attributed to the impact of the level of education. Even if anxiety and depression are confounding factors, and one can speculate that they have interfered with our results, there is no doubt that the educational status is an independent predictor of pain based on the subgroup analysis for age groups.

Nevertheless, our study has limitations. The type of anesthesia and the type of intraoperative analgesia varied from patient to patient and may have influenced our results. Since this effect is limited to the early postoperative period, we did not record the pain intensity and the analgesia requirements during this time to avoid this confounding factor. There is a chance though that more extended effect may have influenced our results. Furthermore, the educational status is just an indicator of the actual cultural and social status of a person. Classifying the patients based on validated questionnaires may have been more appropriate in future similar studies.

Despite these limitations, we believe that the educational status should always be taken in account during the preoperative assessment of the patients. If the patients are of lower education, they should further be assessed for depression and anxiety (e.g With HADS) and if not found to be entirely normal, special effort and extra care should be given in educating them properly and alleviating their psychological distress. This seems to be more useful for young and middle-aged patients.

If we manage to do so, we will likely succeed to compensate for their "handicap", minimize the impact of the lower education on the postoperative pain and allow them to have a better, more comfortable and less stressful recovery from the surgery. Further RCT should be done in order to provide better evidence about the success of this approach.

In conclusion, the educational status may be a significant predictor of postoperative pain due to various reasons, including the poor understanding of the preoperative information, the level of anxiety and depression caused by that and the suboptimal request and use of analgesia. Younger patients (< 40), and patients with subclinical depression are mostly affected while there seems to be no impact on patients over 60 years old.


Demographics and Quantitative Outcomes of the Comparisons of the 3 Educational Status Categories



PO: post operative, VAS: visual analog scale, NRS: numerical rating scale.


Demographics and Qualitative Outcomes of the Comparisons of the 3 Educational Status Categories




Demographics and Quantitative Outcomes of the Comparisons of the 2 Educational Status Categories



Group A = low education, up to junior school; Group B = high-education including high school and university. PO: post operative, VAS: visual analog scale, NRS: numerical rating scale.


Demographics and Qualitative Outcomes of the Comparisons of the 2 Educational Status Categories



Group A = low education, up to junior school; Group B = high-education including high school and university.

  1. Ip, HY, Abrishami, A, Peng, PW, Wong, J, Chung, F. Predictors of postoperative pain and analgesic consumption: a qualitative systematic review. Anesthesiology, 2009;111;657-677.
    Pubmed
  2. Botti, M, Bucknall, T, Manias, E. The problem of postoperative pain: issues for future research. Int J Nurs Pract, 2004;10;257-263.
    Pubmed
  3. Sommer, M, de Rijke, JM, van Kleef, M, Kessels, AG, Peters, ML, Geurts, JW, et al. Predictors of acute postoperative pain after elective surgery. Clin J Pain, 2010;26;87-94.
    Pubmed
  4. Perkins, FM, Kehlet, H. Chronic pain as an outcome of surgery. A review of predictive factors. Anesthesiology, 2000;93;1123-1133.
    Pubmed
  5. Kim, SH, Yoon, KB, Yoon, DM, Kim, CM, Shin, YS. Patient-controlled epidural analgesia with ropivacaine and fentanyl: experience with 2,276 surgical patients. Korean J Pain, 2013;26;39-45.
    Pubmed
  6. Leclerc, A, Gourmelen, J, Chastang, JF, Plouvier, S, Niedhammer, I, Lanoë, JL. Level of education and back pain in France: the role of demographic, lifestyle and physical work factors. Int Arch Occup Environ Health, 2009;82;643-652.
    Pubmed
  7. Chia, YY, Chow, LH, Hung, CC, Liu, K, Ger, LP, Wang, PN. Gender and pain upon movement are associated with the requirements for postoperative patient-controlled iv analgesia: a prospective survey of 2,298 Chinese patients. Can J Anaesth, 2002;49;249-255.
    Pubmed
  8. Lau, H, Patil, NG. Acute pain after endoscopic totally extraperitoneal (TEP) inguinal hernioplasty: multivariate analysis of predictive factors. Surg Endosc, 2004;18;92-96.
    Pubmed
  9. Taenzer, P, Melzack, R, Jeans, ME. Influence of psychological factors on postoperative pain, mood and analgesic requirements. Pain, 1986;24;331-342.
    Pubmed
  10. Jho, HJ, Myung, SK, Chang, YJ, Kim, DH, Ko, DH. Efficacy of pain education in cancer patients: a meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. Support Care Cancer, 2013;21;1963-1971.
    Pubmed
  11. Allard, P, Maunsell, E, Labbé, J, Dorval, M. Educational interventions to improve cancer pain control: a systematic review. J Palliat Med, 2001;4;191-203.
    Pubmed
  12. Louw, A, Diener, I, Butler, DS, Puentedura, EJ. Preoperative education addressing postoperative pain in total joint arthroplasty: review of content and educational delivery methods. Physiother Theory Pract, 2013;29;175-194.
    Pubmed
  13. Zigmond, AS, Snaith, RP. The hospital anxiety and depression scale. Acta Psychiatr Scand, 1983;67;361-370.
    Pubmed
  14. Bjelland, I, Dahl, AA, Haug, TT, Neckelmann, D. The validity of the hospital anxiety and depression scale. An updated literature review. J Psychosom Res, 2002;52;69-77.
    Pubmed
  15. Hawker, GA, Mian, S, Kendzerska, T, French, M. Measures of adult pain: Visual Analog Scale for Pain (VAS Pain), Numeric Rating Scale for Pain (NRS Pain), McGill Pain Questionnaire (MPQ), Short-Form McGill Pain Questionnaire (SF-MPQ), Chronic Pain Grade Scale (CPGS), Short Form-36 Bodily Pain Scale (SF-36 BPS), and Measure of Intermittent and Constant Osteoarthritis Pain (ICOAP). Arthritis Care Res (Hoboken), 2011;63;S240-S252.
    Pubmed
  16. Barbareschi, G, Sanderman, R, Leegte, IL, van Veldhuisen, DJ, Jaarsma, T. Educational level and the quality of life of heart failure patients: a longitudinal study. J Card Fail, 2011;17;47-53.
    Pubmed
  17. Chazelle, E, Lemogne, C, Morgan, K, Kelleher, CC, Chastang, JF, Niedhammer, I. Explanations of educational differences in major depression and generalised anxiety disorder in the Irish population. J Affect Disord, 2011;134;304-314.
    Pubmed
  18. Lorant, V, Deliège, D, Eaton, W, Robert, A, Philippot, P, Ansseau, M. Socioeconomic inequalities in depression: a meta-analysis. Am J Epidemiol, 2003;157;98-112.
    Pubmed
  19. Fryers, T, Melzer, D, Jenkins, R. Social inequalities and the common mental disorders: a systematic review of the evidence. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol, 2003;38;229-237.
    Pubmed
  20. Bjelland, I, Krokstad, S, Mykletun, A, Dahl, AA, Tell, GS, Tambs, K. Does a higher educational level protect against anxiety and depression? The HUNT study. Soc Sci Med, 2008;66;1334-1345.
    Pubmed
  21. Oshodi, TO. The impact of preoperative education on postoperative pain. Part 1. Br J Nurs, 2007;16;706-710.
    Pubmed
  22. Bowers, KS. Pain, anxiety, and perceived control. J Consult Clin Psychol, 1968;32;596-602.
    Pubmed
  23. Krohne, HW, Slangen, K, Kleemannb, PP. Coping variables as predictors of perioperative emotional states and adjustment. Psychol Health, 1996;11;315-330.
  24. Sowden, AJ, Forbes, C, Entwistle, V, Watt, I. Informing, communicating and sharing decisions with people who have cancer. Qual Health Care, 2001;10;193-196.
    Pubmed
  25. Oshodi, TO. The impact of preoperative education on postoperative pain. Part 2. Br J Nurs, 2007;16;790-797.
    Pubmed