The Impact of Educational Status on the Postoperative Perception of Pain
Sophocles Lanitis, Christina Mimigianni, Demetris Raptis, Gionous Sourtse, George Sgourakis
Abstract
Background
Postoperative (PO) pain interferes with the recovery and mobilization of the surgical patients. The impact of the educational status has not been studied adequately up to now.
Methods
This prospective study involved 400 consecutive general surgery patients. Various factors known to be associated with the perception of pain including the educational status were recorded as was the preoperative and postoperative pain and the analgesia requirements for the 1st PO week. Based on the educational status, we classified the patients in 3 groups and we compared these groups for the main outcomes: i.e. PO pain and PO analgesia.
Results
There were 145 patients of lower education (junior school), 150 patients of high education (high school) and 101 of higher education (university). Patients of lower education were found to experience more pain than patients of higher education in all postoperative days (from the 2nd to the 6th). No difference was identified in the type and quantity of the analgesia used. The subgroup analysis showed that patients with depression and young patients (< 40 years) had the maximum effect.
Conclusions
The educational status may be a significant predictor of postoperative pain due to various reasons, including the poor understanding of the preoperative information, the level of anxiety and depression caused by that and the suboptimal request and use of analgesia. Younger patients (< 40), and patients with subclinical depression are mostly affected while there is no impact on patients over 60 years old.
INTRODUCTION
Pain is one of the most common symptoms affecting surgical patients during the postoperative period. Since pain is very subjective and a highly personal experience, it is affected by many factors related to each individual's special characteristics [1]. Despite the evolution of anesthesia and analgesics, one third of the patients still complained about moderate to severe pain after surgery [23]. Effectively controlling the postoperative pain is desirable for various reasons. The postoperative course of the patients should be as comfortable as possible and cause the least possible distress. Failure to adequately control pain may lead to unfavorable postoperative course, long hospitalization, and even chronic pain [345].
Many efforts have been made to identify predictors of postoperative pain [13]. Unfortunately, the factor of "educational status" has not been studied extensively. It has, however, been found that low education is associated with a higher incidence of painful conditions [6]. The few studies that assessed the association of the educational status with the postoperative pain were inconclusive. They either showed no association [78] or in one case a negative association ( i.e. the higher the education, the lower the pain) but this was based only on 40 patients [9]. To our knowledge though, there is no study that actually addressed directly this issue since the aforementioned studies had a different aim. Moreover, we are not aware of any other study performed in a large number of general surgery patients like ours.
On the other hand, a lot of effort has been made to educate the patients preoperatively about the operation and the associated sequellae in order to effectively control the pain. Even in cancer patients, this approach seems to be effective as this was shown in a recent meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials (RCT) [10]. Similar results were presented previously by others [11]. Generally speaking, the structured preoperative education has been found to have a beneficial effect on postoperative pain, anxiety, and recovery; hence, this approach has been used extensively to alleviate the postoperative complications [12]. Various methods of education have been used in an attempt to universally educate the patients before the operation, and these depend both on the delivery method and the ability of the recipient comprehend and interpret the necessary information.
We believe that the educational status of the patients is a significant predictor of pain. In the absence of sufficient evidence, we designed this study in order to assess if there is indeed an association, and which other factors should be taken into account preoperatively in order to have the optimal results in terms of pain management. Our aim was to identify the group of patients at risk for a higher pain score, based on their educational status in order to find a way to offset this handicap, if any.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
1. Statistics
The statistical analysis was carried out using SPSS for Windows version 19 software package (Statistical Package for Social sciences; Inc, Chicago, IL).
For categorical variables, we used the Pearson Chi-square test and Fisher's exact test. In tables bigger than 2 × 2, when the null hypothesis for independence was rejected, (i.e. the variables in the x axis were dependent from the variables in the y axis) we compared the observed frequencies with the expected frequencies (standardized residual) for each cell in order to identify which comparisons led to significant outcomes. In order to do this, we compared the size of the standardized residuals to the critical values that correspond to an alpha of 0.05 (±1.96) to identify the observed values which were significantly more or less than the expected values.
For the continuous variable, we used independent sample t-test for equality of means to assess if there were any statistically significant differences between the means of the outcome values.
For comparisons involving more than 2 groups, we used analysis of variance (ANOVA) to identify statistically significant differences between the means of the outcome values of the groups.
Having rejected the Null hypothesis (i.e. statistically significant effect in ANOVA), we further applied follow-up tests (post-hoc) to assess which groups were different from which other groups or to test various other focused hypotheses. The "Tukey's" and "Bonferroni" post hoc tests were used. We considered significant differences those with P < 0.05. Binary logistic regression was used to assess if there were significant predictors of binary outcomes.
RESULTS
1. Depression (this predictor was found to be significant in both the multivariate and the univariate analysis)
When we compared Group A with Group B for patients who did not have depression, we found that there were still significant differences in the weight, height and length of scar but no difference in their weight and the anxiety score. More specifically, patients of Group A were older (66.6 years vs. 50.5 years, P < 0.001), shorter (165.7 cm vs. 169.8 cm, P < 0.001) and had a longer scar (6.9 cm vs. 5.4 cm, P = 0.003). There were no differences in the preoperative and postoperative pain. More people of group B were smokers (41.1% vs. 23.3%, P = 0.002) but otherwise there were no differences in the sex, in the use of analgesics or narcotics before the operation, in the experience of previous operations, in the percentage of urgent operations, in the type of the scar, and in the anxiety score. Furthermore, there were no differences in the type and quantity of the analgesia required postoperatively.
There were no differences in the sex, use of analgesics/narcotics before the operation, smoking, previous surgery, urgent operation, and anxiety. Furthermore, there were no differences in the type and quantity of the analgesia required postoperatively.
2. Anxiety (this predictor was found to be significant only in univariate analysis)
There were more patients with depression in Group A (11.7% vs. 4.5%, P = 0.02) but otherwise no other significant differences were found in the qualitative factors. Moreover, no differences were observed in the type and quantity of the analgesia required postoperatively. Patients of Group A were older (66.4 years vs. 50.2 years, P < 0.001), shorter and with a longer scar (7.6 vs. 5.7, P < 0.001) and had a higher depression score (3.8 vs. 2.8, P = 0.002) but there were no other differences in the quantitative factors. In terms of postoperative pain, there was a higher pain score during the 2nd PO day for Group A (2.7 vs. 2.0, P = 0.026) but otherwise no other differences were observed.
Patients of Group A were older (73 years vs. 52.9 years, P < 0.001), and shorter (161.9 cm vs. 166.2 cm, P = 0.015) but there were not any other differences in the other quantitative factors or in the level of postoperative pain. There were no differences between the groups for any of the qualitative factors including the postoperative use of analgesia.
3. Age (this predictor was found to be significant only in univariate analysis)
Since age was found to be significantly different between the 2 groups, we did a subgroup analysis based on 3 age groups: the patients under 40 years old (N = 84), patients from 40-60 years old (N = 128) and patients over 60 years (N = 188).
There were no significant differences in any of the factors studied hence, no confounding factors. Moreover, there was no difference in the preoperative pain. On the other hand, significant differences were observed between the 2 groups during the postoperative day 2 and 3. More specifically, Group A had a higher pain score on the 2nd PO day (3.7 vs. 1.6, P = 0.005) and on the 3rd PO day (2.4 vs. 1.0, P = 0.016). Moreover, Group A required more and stronger analgesia for the 1st (P = 0.029), 2nd (P = 0.035) and 4th PO days (P = 0.005) while there were no differences for the 3rd, 5th and 6th PO day.
There were no significant differences in any of the factors studied. Moreover, there was no difference in the preoperative pain and on postoperative pain. On the other hand, there were significant differences in the analgesia required. Group A required more and stronger analgesia for the 1st (P = 0.013), 3rd (P = 0.045), 4th (P = 0.038) and 6th PO days (P = 0.027), while there were no differences for the 2nd, and 5th PO day.
There were no significant differences in any of the factors studied. Moreover, there was no difference in the preoperative pain, postoperative pain or the analgesia required.
DISCUSSION
In this study we found that the educational status is a significant predictor of pain. Moreover, we found that patients who had lower education were more prone to experience depression or anxiety. All these 3 factors seem to be dependent on each other.
Despite the fact that there are no similar studies in the literature (based on general surgery patients), there is data in the literature supporting our findings. More specifically, a study on heart failure patients showed that patients with low education experienced more pain than patients of higher education during the first 6 months [16].
On the other hand, it has been shown that there is indeed a strong association between educational status and mood disorders. A study investigating this association in more than 10,000 people from the general population showed that the relative risk for anxiety was significantly higher for persons with lower education and this was more pronounced in females. Moreover, the same was true for depression where it was found that the lower the education, the higher the relative risk [17]. In the aforementioned study, people of lower education were found to have differences in their socioeconomic status and the material factors. The differences in the socioeconomic status (SES) in patients with lower education are most likely responsible for the higher incidence of mood disorders in this population.
Low SES has been constantly related to mood disorders [1819]. Nevertheless, education is an independent factor influencing rather than being the result of the SES, since it is offered free in most countries (at least in Greece) and precedes occupation and income, which are factors influencing the SES thereafter. Obtaining a good education increases the odds to succeed in western society and achieve goals associated with good emotional well being. Studies have shown that there is a cumulative protective effect of high education on mood disorders, especially depression, and a subsequent advantage on physical health. The HUNT study investigated if the higher education protects against anxiety and depression. In this study, like us, they used the HADS to assess the mood conditions. It was shown that the lowest educational level was associated with higher anxiety/depression scores and this is in line with our findings [20]. The association of low education with anxiety is important since anxiety, when excessive, may have a negative impact on the course and recovery of the surgical patients. It has been widely supported that the anxiety about the anticipated pain and the feeling that there is lack of control of that, sensitizes the individual to the pain stimuli by increasing the reactive pain [2122]. It has been shown that gaining control or understanding better the anticipated condition leads to better adaptation and improved tolerance of it, since the condition may be rendered less threatening. This can be achieved by properly educating the patients and depends on the level of education and the way the medical personnel approaches the patients. Educating patients about the medical actions that will take place should not be a passive process with no confirmation of whether the information is comprehended. On the contrary, it should be an interactive process, with confirmation that learning has taken place [21]. Nevertheless, the patients can be classified in two categories according to the desired level of preoperative education and the amount of information they want to be given: the vigilant and the avoidant [23]. Therefore, information should be tailored to suit each individual's general level of understanding, education and cultural background [24]. Eventually, patients who understand more about their condition will comply with their care needs and will facilitate their recovery and minimize the hospitalization need [21].
A systematic review on the impact of the preoperative education on the postoperative pain showed that this intervention is indeed effective but none of the studies compared the effects of preoperative education and 'no form of education,' and therefore the actual impact may have been underestimated. Nevertheless, sufficient evidence was found to support the effectiveness of preoperative patient education in the improvement of postoperative outcomes [25].
In our study it is important that there was no difference in the preoperative pain between the groups and that the groups were not different in most of the known confounding factors such as sex, use of narcotics/analgesics, previous experience with surgery, urgency of the operation and weight. Patients of lower education experienced more pain than the rest of the patients during the 1st PO week. There was a correlation of the educational status with psychological factors; i.e. anxiety and depression which are known predictors of postoperative pain. It is possible that these factors may have contributed to the results of our study. Indeed, when we did a subgroup analysis including only the patients who did not score high enough for depression (normal), the differences in the intensity of pain between the 2 groups were eliminated. In this subgroup analysis the difference in the mean age between the 2 groups was still significant as was the length of the scar. It is important that there were no differences in the anxiety level which could have acted as a confounding factor. Based on these findings, we may conclude that the educational status is not a "handicap" if we manage to alleviate or control the "subclinical" depression of these patients, and this should be our aim as clinicians. This can be achieved with a proper structured preoperative education and a psychiatric consultation for those patients who score high in the HADS.
On the other hand, when we included only the patients who had a level of depression, the lower education group had a higher pain score on the PO day 2 onwards and this matched the overall results of the analysis. Based on this, we may assume that patients who have lower education cannot effectively manage their subclinical depression during the perioperative period, as opposed to patients of higher education. On the other hand, if the patients have no psychological issues, the educational status has no impact on the perception of pain. When we did a subgroup analysis based on the state of anxiety, we did not find any further differences between the groups, hence the anxiety only mildly affected the results.
Based on the fact that one out of four patients with a lower level of education had some form of depression, this should be taken into account when trying to educate the patients preoperatively.
Although all patients were informed about their operation in the same way by the same trained medical personnel, it is possible that patients of lower education either failed to correctly interpret the information given or hesitated to ask for clarifications when in doubt. This may explain the fact that despite the fact that they had higher pain scores, they did not receive additional or stronger analgesia available on demand by the nursing staff.
Finally, age is known to influence the perception of pain and indeed there were differences between the educational groups. When we stratified the patients according to their age we found that the impact of educational status was very pronounced in the younger patients (i.e. < 40 years), less pronounced in the middle age group (40-60 years) and was eliminated in the elderly (> 60 years). Interestingly when we stratified the patients in 3 age groups, all differences between the groups in all confounding factors were eliminated and the two groups were perfectly matched. Henceforth, in this subgroup analysis all differences found in the main outcomes can only be attributed to the impact of the level of education. Even if anxiety and depression are confounding factors, and one can speculate that they have interfered with our results, there is no doubt that the educational status is an independent predictor of pain based on the subgroup analysis for age groups.
Nevertheless, our study has limitations. The type of anesthesia and the type of intraoperative analgesia varied from patient to patient and may have influenced our results. Since this effect is limited to the early postoperative period, we did not record the pain intensity and the analgesia requirements during this time to avoid this confounding factor. There is a chance though that more extended effect may have influenced our results. Furthermore, the educational status is just an indicator of the actual cultural and social status of a person. Classifying the patients based on validated questionnaires may have been more appropriate in future similar studies.
Despite these limitations, we believe that the educational status should always be taken in account during the preoperative assessment of the patients. If the patients are of lower education, they should further be assessed for depression and anxiety (e.g With HADS) and if not found to be entirely normal, special effort and extra care should be given in educating them properly and alleviating their psychological distress. This seems to be more useful for young and middle-aged patients.
If we manage to do so, we will likely succeed to compensate for their "handicap", minimize the impact of the lower education on the postoperative pain and allow them to have a better, more comfortable and less stressful recovery from the surgery. Further RCT should be done in order to provide better evidence about the success of this approach.
In conclusion, the educational status may be a significant predictor of postoperative pain due to various reasons, including the poor understanding of the preoperative information, the level of anxiety and depression caused by that and the suboptimal request and use of analgesia. Younger patients (< 40), and patients with subclinical depression are mostly affected while there seems to be no impact on patients over 60 years old.
Article information
Articles from The Korean Journal of Pain are provided here courtesy of The Korean Journal of Pain
References
- Ip HY, Abrishami A, Peng PW, Wong J, Chung F. Predictors of postoperative pain and analgesic consumption: a qualitative systematic review. Anesthesiology 2009; 111: 657-677.
- Botti M, Bucknall T, Manias E. The problem of postoperative pain: issues for future research. Int J Nurs Pract 2004; 10: 257-263.
- Sommer M, de Rijke JM, van Kleef M, Kessels AG, Peters ML, Geurts JW, et al. Predictors of acute postoperative pain after elective surgery. Clin J Pain 2010; 26: 87-94.
- Perkins FM, Kehlet H. Chronic pain as an outcome of surgery. A review of predictive factors. Anesthesiology 2000; 93: 1123-1133.
- Kim SH, Yoon KB, Yoon DM, Kim CM, Shin YS. Patient-controlled epidural analgesia with ropivacaine and fentanyl: experience with 2,276 surgical patients. Korean J Pain 2013; 26: 39-45.
- Leclerc A, Gourmelen J, Chastang JF, Plouvier S, Niedhammer I, Lanoë JL. Level of education and back pain in France: the role of demographic, lifestyle and physical work factors. Int Arch Occup Environ Health 2009; 82: 643-652.
- Chia YY, Chow LH, Hung CC, Liu K, Ger LP, Wang PN. Gender and pain upon movement are associated with the requirements for postoperative patient-controlled iv analgesia: a prospective survey of 2,298 Chinese patients. Can J Anaesth 2002; 49: 249-255.
- Lau H, Patil NG. Acute pain after endoscopic totally extraperitoneal (TEP) inguinal hernioplasty: multivariate analysis of predictive factors. Surg Endosc 2004; 18: 92-96.
- Taenzer P, Melzack R, Jeans ME. Influence of psychological factors on postoperative pain, mood and analgesic requirements. Pain 1986; 24: 331-342.
- Jho HJ, Myung SK, Chang YJ, Kim DH, Ko DH. Efficacy of pain education in cancer patients: a meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. Support Care Cancer 2013; 21: 1963-1971.
- Allard P, Maunsell E, Labbé J, Dorval M. Educational interventions to improve cancer pain control: a systematic review. J Palliat Med 2001; 4: 191-203.
- Louw A, Diener I, Butler DS, Puentedura EJ. Preoperative education addressing postoperative pain in total joint arthroplasty: review of content and educational delivery methods. Physiother Theory Pract 2013; 29: 175-194.
- Zigmond AS, Snaith RP. The hospital anxiety and depression scale. Acta Psychiatr Scand 1983; 67: 361-370.
- Bjelland I, Dahl AA, Haug TT, Neckelmann D. The validity of the hospital anxiety and depression scale. An updated literature review. J Psychosom Res 2002; 52: 69-77.
- Hawker GA, Mian S, Kendzerska T, French M. Measures of adult pain: Visual Analog Scale for Pain (VAS Pain), Numeric Rating Scale for Pain (NRS Pain), McGill Pain Questionnaire (MPQ), Short-Form McGill Pain Questionnaire (SF-MPQ), Chronic Pain Grade Scale (CPGS), Short Form-36 Bodily Pain Scale (SF-36 BPS), and Measure of Intermittent and Constant Osteoarthritis Pain (ICOAP). Arthritis Care Res (Hoboken) 2011; 63(Suppl 11): S240-S252.
- Barbareschi G, Sanderman R, Leegte IL, van Veldhuisen DJ, Jaarsma T. Educational level and the quality of life of heart failure patients: a longitudinal study. J Card Fail 2011; 17: 47-53.
- Chazelle E, Lemogne C, Morgan K, Kelleher CC, Chastang JF, Niedhammer I. Explanations of educational differences in major depression and generalised anxiety disorder in the Irish population. J Affect Disord 2011; 134: 304-314.
- Lorant V, Deliège D, Eaton W, Robert A, Philippot P, Ansseau M. Socioeconomic inequalities in depression: a meta-analysis. Am J Epidemiol 2003; 157: 98-112.
- Fryers T, Melzer D, Jenkins R. Social inequalities and the common mental disorders: a systematic review of the evidence. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 2003; 38: 229-237.
- Bjelland I, Krokstad S, Mykletun A, Dahl AA, Tell GS, Tambs K. Does a higher educational level protect against anxiety and depression? The HUNT study. Soc Sci Med 2008; 66: 1334-1345.
- Oshodi TO. The impact of preoperative education on postoperative pain. Part 1. Br J Nurs 2007; 16: 706-710.
- Bowers KS. Pain, anxiety, and perceived control. J Consult Clin Psychol 1968; 32: 596-602.
- Krohne HW, Slangen K, Kleemannb PP. Coping variables as predictors of perioperative emotional states and adjustment. Psychol Health 1996; 11: 315-330.
- Sowden AJ, Forbes C, Entwistle V, Watt I. Informing, communicating and sharing decisions with people who have cancer. Qual Health Care 2001; 10: 193-196.
- Oshodi TO. The impact of preoperative education on postoperative pain. Part 2. Br J Nurs 2007; 16: 790-797.